-->

Developing Psycholinguistic Models of Subject-verb Agreement in Speech Production

Post a Comment



MUH. DANIAL
20401109064
PBI 4
Many writers struggle with subject-verb agreement errors because they misidentify the subject of a sentence. This mistake leads to yet another one--choosing a plural verb based on an incorrectly chosen plural subject. The real subject is “hidden” somewhere in the sentence; it is actually singular. While most writers understand that a singular subject must have a singular verb and that a plural subject must have a plural verb, most do not understand the critical importance of locating the correct subject. Therefore, before concerning yourself with deciding if the verb should be plural or singular, decide if you have located the real subject of the sentence. To locate the subject more easily, try asking yourself the following question: Who or what is performing the action of the verb? Also keep in mind that nouns found in prepositional phrases are never subjects of the whole sentence.

Subject-verb agreement is assumed to be the marking of the verb in an utterance
as determined by properties of the subject. Psycholinguistic models of agreement
in speech production differ as to whether they treat this phenomenon as driven
primarily by syntactic processes or semantic influences. But these models are
based primarily on research in Indo-European languages. This paper suggests
that a useful approach to investigating the psycholinguistic mechanisms behind
agreement in speech production is to extend the research to more typologically
variant languages and more complex structures.

The study of sentence production investigates how speakers produce
grammatically well-formed utterances that communicate an intended message.
The successful production of an utterance entails that, during grammatical
encoding, the speaker must match not only lexical and morphological items to
conceptual information from the message she intends to convey, but also that each
of these items are compatibly integrated in a conventional syntactic structure that
can then be phonologically encoded. As with other areas of psycholinguistics,
research into sentence production is informed by data provided first through
observation of the linguistic phenomenon in question and then experimental
investigation and modeling of the phenomena under examination. As it is
assumed that the psychological mechanisms involved in speech production are the
same for all normal speakers, theories of these mechanisms must account for data
observed in a wide range of typologically variant languages.
So what is “agreement”? In theoretical linguistics, agreement is typically
understood as an asymmetric syntactic relationship in which the form of one
element (the “target”) in a sentence corresponds to the form of another (the
“controller”) (Corbett 2006). Typical examples include number marking on verbs
to correspond with the number of the subject, as in the English examples (1-2).
(1) The cat (sg) plays (sg)
(2) The cats (pl) play (pl)
Other features often considered as reflecting agreement in subject-verb relations
include person and gender (but see Corbett 2006:133-5 for discussion).

Within psycholinguistic studies of agreement production, one main
question concerns the extent to which agreement morphology is influenced by
information in the conceptual representation of the message rather than being
strictly the result of syntactic procedures as defined by a language’s grammar. In
other words, do targets (verbs) “look into” the conceptual message to access the
notional values of agreement features, such as whether or not the referent is
conceived of as singular or plural with regard to the number feature, or do they
simply copy the grammatical values from the corresponding controlling elements
(subjects) in the sentence, that is, whether or not the lexical item referring to the
controlling element is specified as singular or plural1? Thus, data of interest to
studies of agreement production often include examples in which there is a
mismatch between the notional value of the feature and the grammatical value of
the feature. Example of such mismatch with regard to the number feature include the English noun scissors, in which the referent is notionally singular but
grammatically plural, or family, which is grammatically singular but may, in some
dialects, have a notionally plural value (being conceived of as a set of indivual
members). Cases in which agreement morphology reflect the grammatical number
of the controlling referent (the scissors ARE) are taken to be evidence for
agreement production being governed by syntactic processes. On the other hand,
when agreement morphology reflects the notional value of the controlling referent
(the family ARE, in some dialects), we have evidence that conceptual information
is relevant to the agreement production process.
On one side of the debate are production models that describe agreement
as being driven primarily by syntactic procedures. One such model is the Marking
and Morphing model (Eberhard, Cutting & Bock 2005). The Marking and
Morphing model assumes a grammatical encoding process that includes roughly
two components: functional assembly, during which lexical entries are accessed
and matched to grammatical functions as marked by the conceptual message, and
structural integration, at which point agreement morphology is added to the
lexical forms that have been accessed, and those forms are integrated into the
appropriate constituent structure. Agreement processes operate under syntactic
guidance with respect to hierarchical representations of sentence structure, where
features are transmitted or copied from the controller to the target. During
subject-verb agreement production the agreement target (verb) has no access to
the conceptual representation of the controlling referent, but only to the
grammatical value of the features as marked on the lexical form (the subject noun
phrase, after it is encoded lexically).
For  the conclusion, Subject-verb agreement is assumed to be the marking of the verb in an utterance as determined by properties of the subject. Psycholinguistic models of agreement
in speech production differ as to whether they treat this phenomenon as driven
primarily by syntactic processes or semantic influences. But these models are
based primarily on research in Indo-European languages. This paper suggests
that a useful approach to investigating the psycholinguistic mechanisms behind
agreement in speech production is to extend the research to more typologically
variant languages and more complex structures.




References

Berg, Thomas. 1998. “The Resolution of Number Conflicts in English and
German Agreement Patterns.” Linguistics 36(1): 41-70.
Bock, Kathryn, & Carol A. Miller. 1991. “Broken Agreement.” Cognitive
Psychology 23(1): 45-93.
Bock, Kathryn, & J. Cooper Cutting. 1992. “Regulating Mental Energy:
Performance Units in Language Production.” Journal of Memory and
Language 31(1): 99-127.

Related Posts

Post a Comment

Artikel Menarik

Subscribe Our Newsletter